Fish Toxicology and Behavior Research

Behavioral Avoidance of Fluoranthene by Fathead Minnows
 
Introduction
Material and Methods
Results and Discussion
Literature Cited
 

Introduction

Polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons (PAHs) are nearly ubiquitous trace elements in marine and freshwater ecosystems (Neff and Anderson, 1981).   They are composed of two or more fused benzene rings through the sharing of a pair of carbon atoms.   PAHs can be naturally formed by high temperature pyrolysis of organic material, low to moderate temperature diagenesis of sedimentary organic material to form fossil fuels, and direct biosynthesis by microbes and plants (Shabad, 1980; Neff and Anderson, 1981; Dipple, 1983).   In addition, humans add to this environmental load through the inefficient combustion of carbonaceous material from such industrial activities as oil refinery operations, incineration of industrial and domestic waste, and power generation from fossil fuels (Andelman and Suess, 1970; Neff and Anderson, 1981; Dipple, 1983).   PAHs can then reach the aquatic environment through industrial and domestic sewage effluent, surface runoff from land, deposition of airborne particulates, and spillage of petroleum and petroleum products into bodies of water (Neff and Anderson, 1981).   Indeed, the presence of PAHs in the environment is worldwide, and total PAH concentrations can range from 0.001 µg/l to 0.01 µg/l in ground water (Borneff and Kunte, 1964) and from 0.025 µg/l to 3.0 µg/l in polluted areas such as the Great Lakes (Neff, 1979; Eisenreich et al., 1981).       

Upon entering the water, PAHs quickly adhere to organic and inorganic particulate matter.   Much of the particulate PAH is then deposited in bottom sediments.   Leaching or biological activity of these sediments may return a small fraction of the sediment PAH to the water column (Neff and Anderson, 1981).   Extensive research on the uptake, metabolism, and bioconcentration of some PAHs in fish has shown that PAHs are readily accumulated by aquatic biota to levels higher than those in the water.   Aquatic organisms are able to accumulate PAHs at low concentrations in the water column, food, or sediment because PAHs are highly hydrophobic and lipophilic (Lee et al., 1972; Roubal et al., 1977; Neff and Anderson, 1981; Solbakken et al., 1984; Tan and Melius, 1986).   This favors the rapid transfer of PAHs, via the gills and/or the gut, from the aqueous phase to lipophilic compartments such as the brain, the liver, the gallbladder, the gonads, and the flesh (Roubal et al., 1977; Varanasi and Gmur, 1981; Hose et al., 1984).  

Some evidence indicates that PAHs have deleterious effects on fish species.   For example, Hose et al. (1984) documented depressed mitotic rates in the retina and brain, and skeletal malformations in the skull and vertebral column of alevins of rainbow trout ( Oncorhyncus mykiss ) reared in 0.21 to 1.48 µg benzo(a)pyrene/l   .   In another study, insufficient yolk sacs, lack of body pigment, and abnormalities or absence of the eyes were found in rainbow trout alevins reared in 0.21 to 2.99 µg benzo(a)pyrene/l   (Hannah et al., 1982).   Decreased reproductive output (number of eggs laid) was observed in fathead minnows ( Pimephales promelas ) exposed to 6 and 12 µg anthracene/l   (Hall and Oris, 1991).   Fry maternally exposed to these concentrations of anthracene, with subsequent exposure to solar ultraviolet radiation, exhibited developmental   effects such as internal hemorrhaging, edema, and eye and yolk deformities (Hall and Oris, 1991).

While behavioral studies are a particularly promising means of detecting sub lethal effects of contaminants (Little et al., 1982), the behavioral effects of PAH exposure have not been documented.   A method commonly used to assess the behavioral effects of contaminants is to expose fish to plumes of the contaminant and record their preference or avoidance of the plume.   These experiments theoretically have direct relevance to the health of wild populations of fish since fish should have a better opportunity to survive if they could avoid toxicants in their environment (Hansen, 1969).   The resulting pollutant-mediated behavioral modifications can be used to predict the impact on the long term survival of an individual or population (Neff and Anderson, 1981).   The ability of fish to behaviorally avoid various pollutants has been demonstrated by several investigators.   Investigators have found that several species of fishes can avoid plumes of a variety of heavy metals (Sprague, 1964; Sprague et al., 1965; Solbe and Flook, 1975; Cairns et al., 1982; Steele, 1986; Hartwell et al., 1987a,b).   In laboratory tests, green sunfish ( Lepomis cyanellus ) avoided the pesticide chlordane but not lindane (Summerfelt and Lewis, 1967).   Also, fathead minnows avoided a water soluble fraction of coal liquid containing at least 1.7 and 3.5 mg total phenols/l   (Dauble et al., 1985).   The preference or avoidance of PAHs has not been documented but it has been suggested that petroleum hydrocarbons may attract aquatic organisms (Atema, 1976).  

The purpose of this series of experiments was to examine the behavioral responses (preference or avoidance) of fathead minnows to plumes of fluoranthene in a multiple-choice test arena.   Secondarily, we also attempted to determine if experimental pre-exposure to levels of fluoranthene sufficient to kill 85% of a population could have an effect on the behavioral responses of the survivors to fluoranthene plumes.

 

Materials and Methods

Experimental Animals

Immature fathead minnows (Kurtz Hatchery, Elverson, P A) were group housed in flow-through aquaria in charcoalfiltered, dechlorinated water at 24.3 +/- 1.2 C (Table 1) under a I6L: 8D cycle. The fish were fed Biodiet starter moist chow morning and night and frozen brine shrimp at mid-day.

These fish were divided into two experimental groups: Prior to behavioral testing, one group of 1000 fish was exposed for 5 days to an average concentration of 87.5 J.Lg fluoranthene/l. Because the toxicity of some PAHs has been shown to be enhanced by exposure to environmental levels of ultraviolet light (type A), these fish were also continuously exposed to 70 uW / cm2 UV A throughout the course of the 5-day exposure (8,23,24). The survivors (n = 150), hereafter referred to as "dosed," were then placed in clean water. Two to 4 weeks later, these survivors were behaviorally tested as described next. The second group of fish, hereafter referred to as "naive," was exposed to similar environmental conditions except they were not exposed to fluoranthene.

Behavioral Monitoring System

The behavioral monitoring system consisted of a multiplechoice test arena (octagonal fluviarium), a water delivery system, and an associated video-based data acquisition system (Fig. 1). This system allowed the creations of a discrete plume of toxicant and the assessment of preference and avoidance responses of fathead minnows to water-born, chemical stimuli.

The octagonal fluviarium was an octagonal Plexiglas tank incompletely partitioned into eight equal radial octants by central and peripheral walls, leaving a central octagonal swim chamber in which an animal could move without restriction through all eight oct ants (27,35,38,45). A laminar flow of water at I l/min was established through the radial octants by partitioning of the inaccessible central and peripheral portions by stainless steel mesh (250 um mesh size). The integrity of toxicant plumes was verified by food dye plumes and by collecting water samples at various depths within the designated octant and adjacent octants and mapping toxicant concentrations. This mapping showed that at a I 1/min flow through the fluviarium, a toxicant plume could be created in a single octant. Dispersion to adjacent octants was below detectable levels.

The fluviarium was entirely surrounded in black hardware cloth and illuminated with four vita-lights (Duro test Inc., NI, IS watts) and four black-lights (General Electric, USA, 15 watts) mounted in a square configuration above the fluviarium. A Macam Photometries (Livingston, Scotland) Model UV-103 radiometer was used to quantify UV A (320-400 nm). This light configuration minimized shadows in the fluviarium and produced 220-320 uW/cm2 UVA.

Behavioral Bioassay Protocol

Both naive (n = 24) and dosed (n = 24) fish were used. Three experimental subgroups (n = 8 for each) were drawn from each of these two groups. One experimental subgroup of naive fish was then presented with one of three measured concentrations of fluoranthene (mean +/- SE): 14.7 +/- 2.7ug/1, 22.5 +/- 3.1 ug/1, and 43.0 +/-3.9 ug/1. Similarly, one experimental subgroup of dosed fish was presented with one of three concentrations of fluoranthene (mean +/- SE): 8.6 +/- 2.2 u/1, 24.4 +/- 5.8 u/1, and 43.8 +/- 5.3 u/1. Originally, both naive and dosed fish were to be presented with the same concentrations of fluoranthene, but due to fluctuations in fluoranthene concentrations within the fluviarium, there were slight differences in the concentrations presented to the two experimental groups.

All fish were deprived of food 72 h prior to behavioral monitoring, because previous studies have reported that partially satiated animals are not as responsive to chemical cues as unsatiated individuals (7,40). A single fish was tested during each experimental period. Once the fish was put into the fluviarium it was given a lO-min adjustment period to insure active swimming and to reduce the likelihood of "freezing" or "escape" responses due to handling (40). After 10 min, the fish's behavioral activity was recorded and quantified for 15 min (the experimental period) using the Videomex- V system described next. This experimental period was divided into three treatment periods, each 5 min in duration. In the first 5 min of the experimental period, charcoal-filtered, dechlorinated water at 25 +/- 0.5°C (Table I) flowed into all eight octants. This served as the preexposure period and produced baseline behavioral data. During the second 5 min, fluorantheneladen, charcoal-filtered, dechlorinated water flowed into one of the eight octants, the treatment octant, and charcoalfiltered, dechlorinated water flowed into the remaining seven octants (the exposure period). In the last 5 min of the experimental period, again charcoal-filtered, dechlorinated water flowed into all eight octants (the post-exposure period). The octant selected to receive the chemically treated plume was determined systematically such that every other octant (a total of four octants) was used once for each experimental group. This procedure was foHowed to minimize the effects of possible biases of fish for specific octants in the fluviarium due to cues not evident to the investigators (35). The fluviarium was rinsed with IN sodium hydroxide after each experimental subgroup to reduce chemical absorption.

Samples of the fluoranthene-Iaden water plume were collected at low (the floor of the swim chamber) and medium (approximately 2 cm from the floor of the swim chamber) depths, and at the periphery of the swim chamber because the fish spent most of their time there. These samples were stored at -70°C, and later analyzed by reverse-phase high-pressure liquid chromatography to determine the actual concentration to which the fish were exposed. To determine fluoranthene concentrations, 10 uI samples or standards were injected onto a Waters 3.9 mm x IS mBondPak CI8 column at 30C. A mobile phase of 80% acetonitrile and 20% HPLC grade water was used at 0.8 u1/min. A Hitachi Flooo fluorescence detector was used at an excitation of 360 nm and an emission of 460 nm. Peaks were recorded and quantified on an IBM microcomputer based waters 820 Chromatography Data Station.

The fluoranthene-Iaden water was collected immediately prior to experimentation from a once-through elution column (23,24) and then diluted with charcoal-filtered, dechlorinated water to produce the desired concentrations. For the oncethrough elution column, 10 g of fluoranthene was dissolved in 500 ml of acetone and added to 1000 g of dried silica sand (0.2% wt/wt) under a fume hood and allowed to dry of 24 h. The treated sand was then added to a 7 x 50 cm (diameter x length) glass column and the column was placed in-line with 25 C charcoal-filtered, dechlorinated water.

Data Acquisition and Analysis

Movements of the fish were quantified with an automated data collection system consisting of a video-digitizer and associated software (Videomex- V with multiple zone distance traveled monitor, Columbus Instruments, Columbus, OH). The Videomex- V system can quantify slight changes in locomotor activity and record at intervals of 30 s or longer the time that an animal spends in a given number of connecting arenas, the number of entries into and the distance traveled in those arenas, and the speed at which the animal was traveling in each arena. The Videomex- V system eliminates the need for manual frame by frame quantification of movements from video tape. In this study, the Videomex-V system identified the octant position occupied by each fish during each I-min interval of the test. The data set therefore consisted of dependent variables only, since each fish was exposed to all three treatment periods (pre-exposure, exposure, and post-exposure), and the movement of a fish during 1 min was affected by its movement the minute before. With all dependent variables, the data from each experimental group were analyzed using a doubly repeated analysis of variance (ANOVA) (26). This test calculated a separate ANOVA for treatment effects, time effects among treatment periods, and time effects within each treatment period.

Three requirements were established for an acceptable test. First, a fish had to pass through all eight octants by the end of the second minute during any of the three treatment periods. If the fish failed to meet this requirement then it was eliminated from the data set. Only three fish (all dosed fish from the experimental subgroup presented with 24.4 u fluoranthene/1) out of the 48 total fish tested failed to meet this first requirement. Second, there could be no confounding time effects. Pre-exposure behavior could not be statistically different from post-exposure behavior. The test subject had to show recovery to preexposure behavior during the postexposure period. To insure this, a doubly repeated ANOV A was used to compare all three treatment periods (pre-exposure, exposure, and post-exposure) on a per-minute basis for time spent in the treatment octant. Nonsignificant results from this three way comparison verified that there were no time effects among treatment periods or time effects within each treatment period, and therefore any behavioral modifications were concluded to be fluoranthene induced. Finally, if a fish's mean time in the treatment octant exceeded 3 SDs from the mean, it was considered an outlier and removed from the data set. Only two fish (both naive fish from the experimental subgroup presented with 22.5 u fluoranthene/1) were outliers and excluded from data analysis.

Subsequently, to test whether exposure to fluoranthene plumes would cause preference or avoidance, a doubly repeated ANOVA was used to compare the preexposure period to the exposure period, on a per-minute basis. Therefore, all preference and avoidance analyses were two-way comparisons (pre-exposure period to exposure period) only. Significance was declared at p < 0.05.

 

Results and Discussion

Naive fish spent significantly less time in an octant with a fluoranthene plume of 43.0 u/1, 22.5 u/1, or 14.7 u/1 than they did in that octant during the preexposure period, F(I, 7) = 16.21, p = 0.0051; F(I, 5) = 6.78, p = 0.0480; F(I, 7) = 8.73, p = 0.0212, respectively; Fig. 2). Similarly, dosed fish spent significantly less time in an octant with a fluoranthene plume of 43.8 u/1 or 24.4 u/1 than they did in that octant during the pre-exposure period, F(I, 7) = 8.10, p = 0.0248; F(I, 4) = 58.16, p = 0.0016, respectively). No statistically significant change in time spent in an octant between the preexposure and exposure periods was detected in a fluoranthene plume of 8.6 u/1, F(I, 7) = 0.14, p = 0.7205; Fig. 3.

Avoidance responses have been described as very sensitive in studies ofa variety of heavy metals (3,11,12, 30, 32, 34, 35), a water soluble fraction of coal liquid (4), and monocyclic aromatic hydrocarbons (20). Sprague (33) reported a mean avoidance concentration of 5.6 u/1 of zinc for rainbow trout which was only 1.0 % of the lethal threshold concentration. A mean avoidance concentration of 1.5 u/1 of a water soluble fraction of coal liquid was reported for fathead minnows, which was 24% of the lethal concentration (4). Others have reported similar threshold avoidance levels far lower than lethal concentrations (14,43). This sensitivity was even observed for relatively insoluble pollutants such as DDT and cWordane (10,42). In our study the avoidance response of fathead minnows to fluoranthene appeared to be only as sensitive as lethality experiments (Diamond et al., personal communication). From the results on the time spent in a fluoranthene plume, for all six concentrations, the No Observed Effect Concentration for avoidance (NOEC), that concentration of fluoranthene that had no statistically significant behavioral effects on the population, was 8.6 u fluoranthene/1, and the Lowest Observed Effect Concentration for avoidance (LOEC), the lowest concentration of fluoranthene that had a statistically significant effect on the population, was between 8.6 and 14.7 u fluoranthene/1. The NOEC and LOEC for avoidance are comparable to the concentrations that can be found in the environment, which can range from < 1 u/1 to as much as 13 u/1 in polluted areas (15,31). Furthermore, the LOEC for avoidance was similar to the LOEC for survival in 7-day fathead embryo-larval growth and survival tests for fluoranthene, which was between 1 u/1 and 10 u/l (Diamond et aI., personal communication).

A fathead minnow could escape from areas highly contam~ inated with fluoranthene and thus have a better opportunity to survive. However, this may only be a short-term benefit to the individual because displacement from preferred habitats may result in increased mortality through predation, decreased growth, or impaired reproduction (18). In areas where fluoranthene concentrations are below the NOEC, a fathead minnow could not avoid these contaminated areas, and these concentrations would likely prove to be lethal over longer exposures.

Our results clearly demonstrate that both naive and dosed fish can avoid fluoranthene. This avoidance by both naive and dosed fish suggested that pre-exposure did not compromise the fish's ability to detect fluoranthene. Dosed fish retained their ability to detect fluoranthene. The documented avoidance reaction also appeared quickly.

Dauble et aI. (4) concluded that the avoidance of a toxicant during a 2-day exposure would not have been detectable at all individual times. In our study, however, a 5-min observation of fish presented with fluoranthene was sufficient to document an avoidance response and suggests that these fish can quickly detect and avoid fluoranthene.

From the sensitivity of the avoidance response observed in our study, this behavioral monitoring system seems to be a good toxicological tool, and should be used as a standard protocol in aquatic toxicity assessment, along with the traditionallethal and sublethal tests. The concentration and dispersion of the toxicant can be well controlled. With the automated data acquisition system described here, large amounts of data can be obtained for reliable analysis. A wide variety of organisms can be used, and in fact, this behavioral monitoring system has been used to document preference and avoidance responses for zebra fish (Brachydanio rerio), and several species of killifish (27,36,37,39), bullfrog (Rana catesbeiana) and green frog (R. clamitans) tadpoles (38,45, respectively), and crayfish (Procambarus clark;;, Orconectes rusticus, Cambakrus bartoni; 41). Furthermore, these behavioral experiments are nondestructive. They do not use death as an endpoint. They can be conducted with minimal stress to the fish, and potentially allow for retesting of the same individual at later times (19). Also, behavior integrates many cellular processes and is essential to the viability of the organism, the population, and the community (17), therefore, behavioral effects should be tested for their own sake not as a predictor of physiological damage.

 

Literature Cited

Andelman, J.B. and M.J. Suess, 1970.   Polynuclear aromatic hydrocarbons in the water environment.   Bull. Wld. Hlth. Org. 43, 479-508.

Atema, J., 1976.   Sublethal effects of petroleum fractions on the behavior of the lobster, Homarus americanus , and the mud snail, Nassaruis             obsoletus .     In:    Uses, stresses, and adaptations to the estuary, edited by M. Wiley.   Academic Press, New York, N.Y., pp. 302-312.

Borneff, J. and H. Kunte, 1964.   Carcinogenic substances in water and soil.   Part XVI:   Evidence of polynuclear aromatics in water samples through direct extraction.   Arch. Hyg. (Berl.) 148, 585-597.

Cairns, J., Jr., D.S. Cherry and J.D. Giattina, 1982.   Correspondence between behavioral responses of fish in laboratory and heated, chlorinated effluents.   In:   Energy and ecological modeling, edited   by W.J. Mitsch, R.W. Bosserman and J.M. Klopatek.   Elsevier             Scientific Publ. Co., Amsterdam, pp. 207-215.

Dauble, D.D., R.H. Gray, J.R. Skalski, E.W. Lusty and M.A. Simmons, 1985.   Avoidance of a water-soluble fraction of coal liquid by fathead minnows.   Trans. Amer. Fish. Soc.   114, 754-760.

Dipple, A., 1983.   Formation, metabolism, and mechanism of action of polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons.   Cancer Res. (Suppl.)   43, 2422s- 2425s.

Eisenreich, S.J., B.B. Looney and J.D. Thorton, 1981.   Airborne organic contaminants in the Great Lakes ecosystem.   Environ. Sci. Tech. 15, 30-38.

Farr, A.J., C.C. Chabot and D.H. Taylor, 1992.   The effects of temperature and nutritional status in the assessment of preference/avoidance behavior in the fathead minnow.   Midwest Animal Behavior Conference, Springfield, Illinois.

Hall, A.T. and J.T. Oris, 1991.   Anthracene reduces reproductive potential and is maternally transferred during long-term exposure in fathead minnows.   Aquat. Toxicol. 19, 249-264.

Hannah, J.B., J.E. Hose, M.L. Landolt, B.S. Miller, S.P. Felton and W.T. Iwaoka, 1982.   Benzo(a)pyrene-induced morphologic and             developmental abnormalities in rainbow trout.   Arch. Environ. Contam. Toxicol. 11, 727-734.

Hansen, D.J., 1969.   Avoidance of pesticides by untrained sheepshead minnows.   Trans. Amer. Fish. Soc. 98, 426-429.

Hartwell, S.I., D.S. Cherry and J. Cairns, Jr., 1987a.   Avoidance responses             of schooling fathead minnows ( Pimephales promelas ) to a blend of metals during a 9-month exposure.   Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 6, 177-187.

Hartwell, S.I., D.S. Cherry and J. Cairns, Jr., 1987b.   Field validation of avoidance of elevated metals by fathead minnows ( Pimephales promelas ) following in situ acclimation.   Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 6, 189-200.

Hose, J.E., J.B. Hannah, H.W. Puffer and M.L. Landolt, 1984.   Histologic and skeletal abnormalities in benzo(a)pyrene-treated rainbow trout alevins.   Arch. Environ. Contam. Toxicol. 13, 675-684.

Ishio, S., 1965.   Behavior of fish exposed to toxic substances.   Adv. Water Poll. Res. Proc. 2nd Int. Conf. Water Poll. Res., Tokyo, August             1964, Vol. 1.   Pergamon Press, London, pp.19-33.

Kleerekoper, H., 1976.   Effects of sublethal concentrations of pollutants on the behavior of fish.   J. Fish. Res. Board of Can. 33, 2036-2039.

Kynard, B., 1974.   Avoidance behavior of insecticide susceptible and resistant populations of mosquitofish to four insecticides.   Trans. Amer. Fish. Soc. 103, 557-561.

LaFlamme, R.E. and R.A. Hites, 1978.   The global distribution of polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons in recent sediments.   Geochim. Cosmochim. 42, 289-303.

Lee, R.F., R. Sauerheber and G.H. Dobbs, 1972.   Uptake, metabolism, and discharge of polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons by marine fish.   Mar. Biol. 17, 201-208.

Little, E.E., 1990.   Behavioral toxicology:   stimulating challenges for a growing discipline.   Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 9, 1-2.

Little, E.E., B.A. Flerov and N.N. Ruzhinskaya, 1982.   Behavioral             approaches in aquatic toxicity investigations:   A review, Water Quality Section, Amer. Fish, pp. 92-98.

Little, E.E. and S.E. Finger, 1990.   Swimming behavior as an indicator of sublethal toxicity in fish.   Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 9, 13-19.

Maynard, D.J. and D.D. Weber, 1981.   Avoidance reactions of juvenile coho salmon ( Oncorhynchus kisutch ) to monocyclic aromatics.   Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 38, 772-778.

Neff, J.M., 1979.   Polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons in the aquatic environment.   Applied Science Publishers, Ltd., London,   262 pp.

Neff, J.M. and J.W. Anderson, 1981.   Response of marine animals to             petroleum and specific petroleum hydrocarbons.   Applied Science             Publishers, Ltd., London, 177 pp.

Oris, J.T. and J.P. Giesy, Jr., 1985.   The photoenhanced toxicity of             anthracene to juvenile sunfish ( Lepomis spp.). Aquat. Toxicol. 6, 133-146.

Oris, J.T. and J.P. Giesy, Jr., 1986.   Photoinduced toxicity of anthracene to juvenile bluegill sunfish ( Lepomis macrochirus rafinesque):   Photoperiod effects and predictive hazard evaluation.   Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 5, 761-768.

Roubal, W.T., T.K. Collier and D.C. Malins, 1977.   Accumulation and metabolism of carbon-14 labeled benzene, naphthalene, and             anthracene by young coho salmon ( Oncorhynchus kisutch ).   Arch.             Environ. Contam. Toxicol. 5, 513-529.

SAS (Statistical Analysis System), 1989.   SAS user's guide, Version 6.06.     SAS Institute Inc., North Carolina, U.S.A.  

Scarfe, A.D., C.W. Steele and G.K. Rieke, 1985.   Quantitative chemobehavior of fish:   an improved methodology.   Environ. Biol. Fish. 13, 183-194.

Shabad, L.M., 1980.   Circulation of carcinogenic polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons in the human environment and cancer prevention.   J. Natl. Cancer Inst. 64, 405-410.

Solbakken, J.E., S. Tilseth and K.H. Palmork, 1984.   Uptake and elimination of aromatic hydrocarbons and a chlorinated biphenyl in eggs and larvai of cod Gadus morhua .   Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 16, 297-301.

Solbe, J.F. de L.G. and V.A. Flook, 1975.   Studies on the toxicity of zinc sulphate and of cadmium sulphate to stone loach Noemacheilus barbatulus (L.) in hard water.   J. Fish. Biol. 7, 631-637.

Sorrell, R.K., H.J. Brass and R. Reding, 1980.   A review of occurrences and treatment of polynuclear aromatic hydrocarbons in water.               Environ. Intern. 4, 245-254.

Sprague, J.B., 1964.   Avoidance of copper-zinc solutions by young salmon             in the laboratory.   J. Water Poll. Contr. Fed. 36, 990-1004.

Sprague, J.B., 1968.   Avoidance reactions of rainbow trout to zinc sulphate solutions.   Water Res. 2, 367-372.

Sprague, J.B., P.F. Elson and R.L. Saunders, 1965.   Sublethal copper-zinc pollution in a salmon river:   a field and laboratory study.   Air Soil             Water Poll.   9, 531-543.

Steele, C.W., 1986.   Responses of zebra fish, Brachydanio rerio , to             behavior-altering chemicals.   Ph.D dissertation, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX, 220 pp.

Steele, C.W., D.W. Owens, A.D. Scarfe and P. Thomas, 1985.   Behavioral assessment of the sublethal effects of aquatic pollutants.   Mar. Pollut. Bull. 16, 221-224.

Steele, C.W., A.D. Scarfe and D.W. Owens, 1987.   Suppression of a positive feeding response by fishes to alanine by sublethal copper carried in the same water mass.   In:   Heavy metals in the environment, edited by S.E. Lindberg and T.C. Hutchinson.             Edinburgh, pp. 181-183.

Steele, C.W., S. Strickler-Shaw and D.H. Taylor, 1989.   Behavior of tadpoles of the bullfrog, Rana catesbeiana , in response to sublethal lead exposure.   Aquat. Toxicol. 14, 331-344.

Steele, C.W., D.W. Owens and A.D. Scarfe, 1990.   Attraction of zebrafish, Brachydanio rerio , to alanine and its suppression by copper.   J. Fish Biol 36, 341-352.

Steele, C.W., D.H. Taylor and S. Strickler-Shaw, 1993.   Avoidance-             preference testing in aquatic toxicology:   towards a standardized             methodology. In:   Environmental toxicology and risk assessment, edited by J.W. Gorsuch, F.J. Dwyer, C.G. Ingersoll and T.W. La Point. American Society for Testing and Materials, Philadelphia, P.A. (in press).

Steele, C.W., S. Strickler-Shaw and D.H. Taylor, In press.   Attraction of crayfishes, Procambarus clarkii , Orconectes rusticus , and Cambarus             bartoni , to a feeding stimulant and its suppression by a blend of             metals.   Environ. Toxicol. Chem.

Summerfelt, R.C. and W.M. Lewis, 1967.   Repulsion of green fish by certain chemicals.   J. Water Poll Contr. Fed. 39, 2030-2038.

Syazuki, K., 1964.   Studies on the toxic effects of industrial waste on fish and shellfish.   J. Shimonoseki Coll. Fish. 13, 157-211.

Tan, B. and P. Melius, 1986.   Polynuclear aromatic hydrocarbon metabolism in fishes.   Comp. Biochem. Physiol. 83C, 217-224.

Taylor, D.H., C.W. Steele and S. Strickler-Shaw, 1990.   Responses of green frog ( Rana clamitans ) tadpoles to lead-polluted water.   Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 9, 87-93.

Varanasi, U. and D.J. Gmur, 1981.   Hydrocarbons and metabolites in english sole ( Parophrys vetulus ) exposed simultaneously to [ 3 H]benzo[a]pyrene and [ 14 C]naphthalene in oil-contaminated sediment.   Aquat. Toxicol. 1, 49-67.